Information Technology Reference
In-Depth Information
Mokros, J. R., & Tinker, R. F. (1987). The impact
of microcomputer-based labs on children's abil-
ity to interpret graphs. Journal of Research in
Science Teaching , 24 (4), 369-383. doi:10.1002/
tea.3660240408
Reiner, M. (2000). Thought experiments and
embodied cognition . In Gilbert, J. K., & Boul-
ter, C. J. (Eds.), Developing Models in Science
Education (pp. 157-176). Netherlands: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Monaghan, J. M., & Clement, J. (2000). Al-
gorithms, visualization, and mental models:
High school students' interactions with a
relative motion simulation. Journal of Science
Education and Technology , 9 (4), 311-325.
doi:10.1023/A:1009480425377
Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick,
L. B. (2000). Naïve physics reasoning: A com-
mitment to substance-based conceptions. Cogni-
tion and Instruction , 18 (1), 1-34. doi:10.1207/
S1532690XCI1801_01
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. (2007). Does com-
paring solution methods facilitate conceptual and
procedural knowledge? An experimental study on
learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational
Psychology , 99 (3), 561-574. doi:10.1037/0022-
0663.99.3.561
Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current
measures of accuracy of feeling-of knowing
predictions. Psychological Bulletin , 95 , 109-133.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.109
Nemirovsky, R., & Monk, S. (2000). “If you
look at it the other way…”: An exploration into
the nature of symbolizing . In Cobb, P. (Eds.),
Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics
classrooms: Perspectives on Discourse, Tools,
and Instrumental Design (pp. 177-221). New
Jersey: Erlbaum.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. (in press). Compared
to what? The effects of different comparisons on
conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility
for equation solving. Journal of Educational
Psychology .
Roth, W. M., & Lawless, D. V. (2002). Scientific
investigations, metaphorical gestures, and the
emergence of abstract scientific concepts. Learn-
ing and Instruction , 12 , 285-304. doi:10.1016/
S0959-4752(01)00023-8
Nersessian, N. J. (2002). Maxwell and “the Method
of Physical Analogy”: Model-based reasoning,
generic abstraction, and conceptual change. In
D. Malament (Ed.), Essays in the History and
Philosophy of Science and Mathematics (pp.
129-166). Lasalle, Il: Open Court.
Salmon, W. C. (1998). Causality and Ex-
planation . Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1093/0195108647.001.0001
Núñez, R. E., Edwards, L. D., & Matos, J. F.
(1999). Embodied cognition as grounding for
situatedness and context in mathematics educa-
tion. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 39 (1-3),
45-65. doi:10.1023/A:1003759711966
Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (2003). Construc-
tion and interference in learning from multiple
representation. Learning and Instruction , 13 (2),
141-156. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00017-8
Pang, M. F., & Marton, F. (2005). Learning theory
as teaching resource: Enhancing students' un-
derstanding of economic concepts. Instructional
Science , 33 (2), 159-191. doi:10.1007/s11251-
005-2811-0
Schwartz, D., Biswas, G., Bransford, J., Bhuva,
B., Balac, T., & Brophy (2000). Computer tools
that link assessment and instruction: Investigating
what makes electricity hard to learn. In S. Lajoie
(Ed.), Computers as Cognitive Tools, Vol. II (pp.
273-307). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates.
Search WWH ::




Custom Search