Information Technology Reference
In-Depth Information
Konopka, A. E., & Benjamin, A. S. (2009). Schematic knowledge changes what judgments of
learning predict in a source memory task. Memory & Cognition , 37 (1), 42-51.
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation
of memory accuracy. Psychological Review , 103 , 490-517.
Koriat, A., Ma'ayan, H., & Nussinson, R. (2006). The intricate relationships between mon-
itoring and control in metacogntion: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between
subjective experience and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 135 (1),
36-69.
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review , 14 , 219-224.
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Optimizing self-regulated study: The benefits-and costs-of
dropping flashcards. Memory , 16 , 125-136.
Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 32 , 609-622.
Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners' choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory , 17 (5),
493-501.
Lajoie, S. P. (Ed.). (2000). Computers as cognitive tools (vol. 2): No more walls. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M.
Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625-632).
London: Academic Press.
Le Ny, J. F., Denheire, G., & Taillanter, D. (1972). Regulation of study-time and interstimulus
similarity in self-paced learning conditions. Acta Psychologica , 36 , 280-289.
Linn, M. C., Davis, E. A., & Bell, P. (Eds.). (2004). Internet Environments for Science Education.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Liu, X., Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2009). Learning to avoid part-list cues. Manuscript in
preparation.
Liu, X., & Fang, G. (2005). Development of children's strategy on allocation of study time. Acta
Psychologica Sinica , 37 (5), 623-631.
Liu, X., & Fang, G. (2006). Development of elementary school students on allocation of study time
under different time limit. Acta Psychologica Sinica , 38 (3), 365-374.
Lovelace, E. A. (1984). Metamemory: Monitoring future recallability during study. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 10 (4), 756-766.
Maki, R. H., & Serra, M. (1992). The basis of test predictions for text material. The Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 18 , 116-126.
McDaniel, M. A., Blischak, D. M., & Challis, B. (1994). The effects of test expectancy on
processing and memory of prose. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19 (2), 230-248.
Metcalfe, J. (2002). Is study time allocated selectively to a region of proximal learning? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General , 131 , 349-363.
Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation.
Journal of Memory and Language , 52 , 463-477.
Metcalfe, J., Schwartz, B. L., & Joaquim, S. G. (1993). The cue familiarity heuristic in
metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 19 ,
851-861.
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer
appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior , 16 (5), 519-533.
Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental
Psychology , 64 , 482-488.
Neely, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (1981). Test-expectancy and semantic-organization effects in recall
and recognition. Memory & cognition , 9 (3), 283-300.
Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people's judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely
accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed-JOL effect”. Psychological Science ,
2 (4), 267-270.
Search WWH ::




Custom Search