Biomedical Engineering Reference
In-Depth Information
bottom panel of Figure 4.4. Note the variable degree of synchronization, especially
for example C, which has a large increase of synchronization after second 3.
4.5
Conclusion
In this chapter we applied several linear and nonlinear measures of synchronization
to three typical EEG signals. The first measure we described was the cross-correla-
tion function, which is so far the most often used measure of correlation in neurosci-
ence. We then described how to estimate coherence, which gives an estimation of
the linear correlation as a function of the frequency. In comparison to cross correla-
tion, the advantage of coherence is that it is sensitive to correlations in a limited fre-
quency range. The main limitation of cross correlation and coherence is that they
are linear measures and are therefore not sensitive to nonlinear interactions.
Using the information theory framework, we showed how it is possible to have
a nonlinear measure of synchronization by estimating the mutual information
between two signals. However, the main disadvantage of mutual information is that
it is more difficult to compute, especially with short datasets. Finally, we described
phase synchronization measures to quantify the interdependences of the phases
between two signals, irrespective of their amplitudes. The phases can be computed
using either the Hilbert or the wavelet transform, with similar results.
In spite of the different definitions and sensitivity to different characteristics of
the signals of different synchronization methods, we saw that all of these measures
gave convergent results and that naïve estimations based on visual inspection can be
very misleading. It is not possible in general to assert which is the best synchroniza-
tion measure. For example, for very short datasets mutual information may be not
reliable, but it could be very powerful if long datasets are available. Coherence may
be very useful for studying interactions at particular frequency bands, and phase
synchronization may be the measure of choice if one wants to focus on phase rela-
tionships. In summary, the “best measure” depends on the particular data and
questions at hand.
References
[1]
Strogatz, S., Sync: The Emerging Science of Spontaneous Order, New York: Hyperion
Press, 2003.
[2]
Niedermeyer, E., “Epileptic Seizure Disorders,” in Electroencephalography: Basic Princi-
ples, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields, 3rd ed., E. Niedermeyer and F. Lopes Da
Silva, (eds.), Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 1993.
[3]
Engel, A. K., and W. Singer, “Temporal Binding and the Neural Correlates of Sensory
Awareness,” Trends Cogn. Sci., Vol. 5, No. 1, 2001, pp. 16-25.
[4]
Singer, W., and C. M. Gray, “Visual Feature Integration and the Temporal Correlation
Hypothesis,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci., Vol. 18, 1995, pp. 555-586.
[5]
Rieke, F., et al., Spikes: Exploring the Neural Code , Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1997.
[6]
Varela, F., et al., “The Brainweb: Phase Synchronization and Large-Scale Integration,”
Nature Rev. Neurosci., Vol. 2, No. 4, 2001, pp. 229-239.
[7]
van Luijtelaar, G., and A. Coenen, The WAG/Rij Rat Model of Absence Epilepsy: Ten
Years of Research, Nymegen: Nijmegen University Press, 1997.
 
Search WWH ::




Custom Search