Biology Reference
In-Depth Information
that either, do you?” I barely passed and to this day always remember that bewildering
experience when serving on doctoral exams myself.
The dissertation developed into an appraisal of behavioral homology, backed by a
survey of constriction and defense in snakes. I countered Atz's skepticism by observing
that antlers and some other structures are also variable, yet conversely many action
patterns are as stereotyped as bone shapes; moreover, newborn animals often exhib-
it identical behaviors across taxonomic groups. Clearly anatomy isn't fundamentally
unique in those respects, so I would address serpentine feeding and antipredator tactics
with a question that tantalized Darwin, Wallace, and Fitch: Why are there similarities
and differences among species? Since extensive taxonomic sampling in the field would
be impossible given the time constraints of a Ph.D. program, I videotaped constricting
behavior by snakes in the Atlanta, Dallas, Fort Worth, Houston, and Knoxville zoos. I
also observed captives in Gordon's research lab, including an exceedingly rare Oaxacan
dwarf boa sent from Mexico by my friend Jon Campbell, by then a graduate student with
Bill Pyburn.
Almost six hundred observations of seventy-five species showed that constrictors ap-
plied loops around prey in nineteen ways, but forty-eight relatively primitive species al-
ways used only one of them. Baby pipesnakes, boas, and pythons employed the exact
same movements too—no experience required; furthermore, their ancient style persis-
ted in the face of ecological diversification. Green tree pythons, giant aquatic green
anacondas, and the secretive Oaxacan dwarf boa, despite their diverse habitat preferen-
ces, all subdued prey with identical coils, which I proposed had facilitated the origin of a
large gape in some snakes (I'll cover this topic in more detail in chapter 9). Conversely,
antipredator mechanisms, descriptions of which I gleaned from the natural history liter-
ature and my own field studies, have generally evolved more recently and are fine-tuned
to local ecological conditions. One hundred and twenty-four species for which I had ob-
servations defended themselves with tactics that are more reliably predicted by lifestyle
than legacy; arboreal species the world over have independently developed open-mouth
threats, for example, and unrelated burrowers use tail displays. 24
More generally, I'd confirmed the early ethologists' claim that comparative studies
of behavior could be fruitful, and the final defense went of without a hitch. It would
be more than a decade, however, before I began to reflect on how behavioral homology
might inspire nature appreciation.
Those four years at Tennessee contrasted sharply with the orderliness and normality
that characterized my youth. I shared a four-room farmhouse twenty miles north of
Knoxville with my second wife, Dona (we'd married soon after I moved to Knoxville),
and two venomous beaded lizards, two tame opossums, nine cats, a dog, and an African
pygmy goat. Sluggo had vacant golden eyes, spiraled horns, a typical goat penchant for
licking his lavender erection whenever visitors approached our porch, and a resolve to
kill Layla, the Malamute, by slamming her against the house. The larger beaded lizard
was an escape artist; on one nocturnal foray he crawled into a hole in the bathroom
wall, and I had to tear out a piece of plywood behind the sink to retrieve him. One early
morning I awoke with sudden fears of a heart attack, only to find myself nose to nose
with fourteen-pound Posey the possum, asleep on my chest. Throughout that craziness,
Search WWH ::




Custom Search